The Promise of “Optimal Input”
Beniko Mason
Shitennoji University Junior College, EmeritaStephen Krashen
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (2020). The promise of “Optimal Input.” Turkish Online Journal of English Language Teaching (TOJELT), 5(3), 146-155.
University of Southern California, Emeritus
For the last 40+ years, teachers have been advised to avoid following one
specific methodology and use techniques and activities from different
language teaching approaches and methodologies. This approach is called
the “Eclectic Approach,” and almost all the modern course books support
mixing methodologies. (British Council). (1) After 40+ years, however, the
Eclectic Approach has not been shown to be the most effective method.
There have been some objections against using only an acquisition
approach. In this paper, we will explain that they are no longer valid
objections when optimal input methods by way of Story-Listening and
Guided Self-Selected Reading are used in the classrooms. It is possible that
other methods exist, which also provides optimal input that produces
results as strong as SL/GSSR that are convenient and easy to install in the
classroom. This should be investigated, and until this is done, SL/GSSR is
our best bet. It is clearly time to seriously consider the idea of a 100%
acquisition-based program.
For the last 40+ years, teachers have been advised to avoid following one specific methodology and use techniques and activities from different language teaching approaches and methodologies. This approach is called the “Eclectic Approach,” and almost all the modern course books support mixing methodologies. (British Council). (1)
After 40+ years, however, the Eclectic Approach has not been shown to be the most effective method. Studies revealed that the more eclectic teaching included meaningful comprehension-based activities, the better the results (e.g., Isik, 2000). In fact, research done over the last 30 years has shown that “pure” (not mixed) optimal input is not only effective, but also efficient for language development (e.g., Elley & Mangubhai, 1983; Krashen, 2004a, 2011; Mason & Krashen, 1997, 2004; Mason, 2013, 2018; Smith, 2006). Optimal input (Krashen & Mason, 2020) is the cause of effective and efficient (optimal) language acquisition in the limited amount of time we have available to us in schools.
Although optimal input instruction is a good answer, most schools still use the eclectic approach for different reasons, and do not fully exploit the use of the acquisition approach. One popular reason is that the students must take exams and it is thought that the acquisition approach does not prepare students for them. Other reasons given are that students prefer the traditional test preparation course, and that the acquisition approach is slow. These are exactly the same reasons that were given 40+ years ago, discussed below. In this paper, we will explain that these objections are no longer valid and that the acquisition approach using optimal input is a promising answer.
The six objections against fully embracing the acquisition-based approach only were:
- We don’t have the materials
- “Trivialization”
- The “just talk” fallacy
- Personnel problems
- Students’ expectations
- “It takes too long”
(Krashen, 1985; pages 54-58)
RESPONSES TO THE OBJECTIONS
1. “MATERIALS”
When we free ourselves from believing that we must teach according to a grammarbased syllabus, or that we must teach the first basic 2,000 high- frequency words first, and when we understand that language acquisition emerges in a predictable order when students receive rich compelling language input that they understand and enjoy, we can use stories and books for materials instead of traditional textbooks.
There are ways to use stories and books as materials, which are fully consistent with the five hypotheses of Comprehension Theory (Krashen, 1985, 2003). The two approaches that have the greatest promise are Story-Listening and Guided Self-Selected Reading (Mason & Pendergst, 1997; Mason, 2014, 2015, 2019; Krashen & Mason, 2020; Mason & Krashen, 2020). Both ways of teaching appear to provide “Optimal input.” The results of several studies show that they are not only effective; but also efficient for developing listening and reading comprehension, vocabulary, grammar, and writing, as measured by teacher-made tests and standardized tests (Krashen & Mason, 2015; Mason, Krashen, 2017, Mason, 2013, 2018).
2. “TRIVIALIZATION”
Stories and books are not trivial
When input-based teaching consisted primarily of TPR, games, content-related activities, plays, and songs, the critics were right when they complained that the input was trivial, that is, it did not include enough language, enough variety of language, and was not interesting enough, let alone “compelling.”
Stories and books, however, are not trivial; they can provide students with input that is rich, comprehensible, and compelling. There is good evidence showing that reading is the most efficient way to provide comprehensible input for vocabulary acquisition (Nation, 2014).
Hsieh, Wang and Lee (2011) reported that 65 picture books that were used for readaloud to elementary school children in Taiwan contained three times as many words as textbooks used at this level. These children began to read books in the “Marvin Redpost” series (3rd-grade reading level) (2) on their own towards the end of their fourth year at the language school where Wang taught.
Walter (2020) reported that the words that students encountered in 50 Story-Listening lessons given to beginning high school French as a foreign language students were much richer in quality and more abundant in quantity than the words in the textbooks used during the same length of school time.
Stories are written in correct grammar, and they contain a wide variety of vocabulary, including many low-frequency words and academic words (McQuillan, 2019). When students hear stories and read books that they enjoy and understand, they subconsciously acquire many words necessary to go on to the next level. When stories are not just read to the students, but are delivered by a teacher who knows what her students understand and do not understand, students will encounter just the right amount of “i+1” in both the text and the teachers’ spoken language.
3. IT IS NOT “JUST TALK”
Story-Listening Instruction is not Storytelling
When stories are told to students in the Story-Listening Way, it is not “Just talking.” Story-Listening (Mason & Krashen, 2004; Mason, Vanata, Jander, Borsch, & Krashen, 2009; Mason & Krashen, 2018; Clarke, 2019; Mason, Smith, & Krashen, 2020) is more than just telling a story. It uses written texts of fairy/folk tales as materials and uses a strategy called the Comprehension-Aiding Supplementation (Krashen, 1982; Krashen, Mason, & Smith, 2018) in order to make incomprehensible input comprehensible.
The syllabus is flexibly adjusted by the teacher to accept the students’ natural development and interests. The teacher, however, is the one who decides what story to use and what words to use to tell the story. The teacher takes time to explain the content of the story using words that the teacher thinks that the students already know. The teacher knows the problems that the students have with comprehension and knows how to make the stories comprehensible.
Guided Self-Selected Reading is not Extensive Reading
Books are not trivial either, even when the books are pedagogical readers graded according to vocabulary level. Because the grading is not 100% precise, and because the books are on different topics, the graded readers contain some difficult words even when they are labeled as starter level readers.
For example, in one of the 200-headword starter level Penguin graded readers, “Manuscript” is used in the first page in “The Big Bag Mistake,” and “Ungrateful” is used in the 300-headword starter level MacMillan graded reader, “Alissa.” “Ungrateful” is at the 3rd 1,000-word level word, and “Manuscript” is at the 4th 1,000-word level word.
In Guided Self-Selected Reading, students are guided to appropriate books by an experienced teacher with extensive knowledge about the readers. The books that the teacher provides to the students include not only graded readers, but also authentic books that are written for young native speaker readers (McQuillan, 2016). (2)
Unlike Extensive Reading, students are not asked to read widely for general education in a GSSR program, but they are advised to read narrowly (Krashen, 2004b). Students are not taken to the library and are totally on their own. They are not advised to start reading graded readers according to their scores on the placement tests. Students are guided: 1) To the books that they can read easily; 2) To appropriate level books; and 3) To the amount of reading needed in order to reach their goal within the limited time in school.
Students are required to keep a reading log for individual guidance; but there are no comprehension questions (Krashen & Mason, 2019), or vocabulary exercises, and there is no summary writing in English (in the case of the EFL situation), and no tests after reading a book (Mason, 2015). Guided Self-Selected Reading does not combine reading with conscious learning or with Intensive Reading. In GSSR, reading alone is enough. (Krashen, 2009; McQuillan, 2019; Mason, 2019).
4. “NO PERSONNEL PROBLEMS”
Anyone with some training can do it.
Both Story-Listening (SL) and Guided-Self-Selected Reading (GSSR) methods are not difficult to learn and can be used by anyone, even by those without teaching certificates or college degrees. The methods use common sense. They involve techniques that we have used from the beginning of human history.
5. STUDENTS’ EXPECTATIONS
Orientation and positive experiences change their perspective
A frequently heard opposition to the use of acquisition-based methods is that teachers believe that students prefer conscious learning. Our experience is that when students experience that listening to stories and reading books are much easier and more fun and it leads to acquiring the language and better performance on standardized tests, they prefer acquisition-based methods. To help them understand what these methods are, we first give them an orientation and then have them experience language acquisition using the methods.
Orientation sessions cover these topics:
- the theory,
- the methods,
- the positive research evidence,
- the goal of the program,
- how students are guided to reach the goal,
- what students are expected to do, and
- how students will be evaluated.
After they receive the above orientation, they experience language acquisition using SL and GSSR. Once they experience how easy and pleasant this acquisition-based instruction is, they start having hope, even if they are still skeptical about an Input-Only approach.
When students experience immediate success with SL and GSSR, they are motivated to continue. Motivation does not come from understanding the theory and positive research evidence but comes from successful personal experience with the methods.
Once students experience how pleasant hearing stories and reading books are, they prefer this to studying grammar, memorizing vocabulary, doing worksheets, taking tests, and being corrected and evaluated. What’s more, when they see that they are
5
making significant gains on standardized tests often after only one semester or, at most, a year (Mason, 2013, Mason & Krashen, 2019), gains that they had not experienced before with the hard work of using the traditional approach, they are convinced of the value of these methods.
6. EFFICIENCY
Both time and cost-efficient
Listening to comprehensible and highly engaging stories is not only more pleasant, but it is also more efficient for vocabulary acquisition. The results of several studies show that acquisition of vocabulary from Story-Listening proceeds at a faster rate than memorizing words. We have also found that adding traditional post-listening instruction on vocabulary resulted in more words mastered; but was not as efficient in terms of words retained per minute. The time is better spent in listening to and reading stories (Mason and Krashen, 2004; Mason, Vanata, Jander, Borsch, & Krashen, 2009; Mason & Krashen, 2018; Clarke, 2019; Mason, Smith, & Krashen, 2020).
- Constantino (1995) reported that her ESL students who read during the summer passed the TOEFL requirement, but that one who studied for the test did not. • In Mason (2006), Japanese college students who stayed in Japan and read books from the university library gained the same number of points per week on the TOEFL as international students who were in an Intensive English Program at a university campus in the USA.
- A middle-aged Japanese man who read about 6,500 pages gained 180 points on the TOEIC mostly from mostly reading alone (Mason 2011).
- A student who read 1700 pages in one-semester reading books that she checked out from the university library gained 85 points on the TOEIC during that semester while she gained only five points after spending 16- months in Canada staying with a host family and taking college classes (Mason & Krashen, 2019). Reading alone was unbelievably efficient.
CONCLUSION
When we use stories and books as materials in the way of Story-Listening and GSSR, the problems we listed earlier disappear. When the input is interesting and comprehensible, students listen and read. When the input is abundant and it is rich, that is, when it contains language that adds depth and interest to the story and helps make unfamiliar language more comprehensible, it helps provide i+1 for everyone in
the class. Research has shown that the use of stories and books as done in Story-Listening and Guided Self-Selected Reading are not only effective but also time-efficient for language acquisition.
The usual objections to an input-only approach have been answered. It is clearly time to seriously consider the idea of a 100% acquisition-based program.
Notes:
(1) British Council: https://www.teachingenglish.org.uk/article/eclectic-approach
(2) Marvign Redpost: https://www.scholastic.com/teachers/books/marvin-redpost 1-kidnapped-at-birth-by-louis-sachar/ For example: #1 Kidnapped at Birth is at Accelerated Reader Level: 2.8 ( https://www.akjeducation.com/marvin-redpost-1- kidnapped-at-birth-9780679819462)
(3) https://www.ethnologue.com/guides/how-many-languages
Acknowledgement
We thank Ken Smith and John Truscott for helpful discussion.
REFERENCES
Constantino, R. (1995). The effect of pleasure reading: Passing the TOEFL test doesn’t have to hurt. Mosaic 3(1):15-17.
Elley, W. B., & Mangubhai, F. (1983). The impact of reading on second language learning. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 53-67.
Hsieh, M-Y., Wang, F. Y., & Lee, S. Y. (2011). A corpus-based analysis comparing vocabulary input from storybooks and textbooks. The International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching (IJFLT), 25-33, Winter 2011.
Isik, A. (2000). The role of input in second language acquisition: More comprehensible input supported by grammar instruction or more grammar instruction? ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics 129-130: 225-274.
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. New York: Prentice-Hall. Available at:
http://www.sdkrashen.com/content/books/principles_and_practice.pdf Krashen, S. D. (1985). The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. Torrance, CA: Laredo Publishing Company Inc.
Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in language acquisition and use: The Taipei lectures. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Krashen, S. (2004a). The power of reading: Insights from the research. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Krashen, S. (2004b). The case for narrow reading. Language Magazine, 3(5), 17-19, Krashen, S. (2009). Anything but reading. Knowledge Quest, 37(5), 18-25.
Krashen, S. (2011). Free voluntary reading. ABC-CLIO: Libraries Unlimited. Krashen, S., & Mason, B. (2015). Can second language acquirers reach high levels of proficiency through self-selected reading? An attempt to confirm the Nation's (2014) result. The International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 10(2), 10-19.
Krashen, S., & Mason, B. (2019). A note on comprehension checking. Journal of English Language Teaching, 61(1), 23-24.
Krashen, S., & Mason, B. (2020). The optimal input hypothesis: Not all comprehensible input is of equal value. CATESOL Newsletter, May 2020. pages 1-2.
Krashen, S., Mason, B., & Smith, K. (2018). Some new terminology: comprehension aiding supplementation and form-focusing supplementation. Language learning and Teaching, 60(6), 12-13.
Mason, B. (2006). Free Voluntary Reading and Autonomy in Second Language Acquisition: Improving TOEFL Scores from Reading Alone. The
International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching (IJFLT), 2(1), 2-5. Mason, B. (2011). Impressive gains on the TOEIC after one year of comprehensible input, with no output or grammar study. International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching (IJFLT), 7(1), 8-12.
Mason, B. (2013). ‘Efficient use of literature in second language education: Free reading and listening to stories’, In J. Brand & C. Lütge (Eds.), Children’s Literature in Second Language Education (pp. 25-32). London: Continuum.
Mason, B. (2014). Self-Selected Pleasure Reading and Story Listening for Foreign Language Classrooms. Shitennoji University (IBU) Bulletinl, 57. 247-256. Mason, B. (2015). Free voluntary reading with story-listening. Kanagawa-ken, Japan: Seizansha.
Mason, B. (2018). A pure comprehension approach: more effective and efficient than eclectic second language teaching? IBU Journal of Educational Research and Practice, 6, 69-79.
Mason, B. (2019). Guided SSR before self-selected reading. Shitennoji University Kiyo, 67, 445-456.
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (1997a). Extensive reading in English as a foreign language. System, 25(1), 91-102.
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (2004). Is form-focused vocabulary instruction worthwhile? RELC Journal 35(2), 179-185.
Mason, B. & Krashen, S. (2017). Self-selected reading and TOEIC performance: Evidence from case histories. Shitennoji Kiyo, 63, 469-475.
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (2018). American students’ vocabulary acquisition rate in Japanese as a foreign language from listening to a story. Turkish Online Journal of English Language Teaching (TOJELT), 3(1), 6-9.
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (2019). Hypothesis: A class supplying rich comprehensible input is more effective and efficient than “Immersion.” IBU Journal of Educational Research and Practice, 7, 83-89.
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (2020). Story-Listening: A Brief Introduction. CATESOL Newsletter, June.
Mason, B., & Pendergast, T. (1997). Tadoku program at International Buddhist University. Language Teacher, 21(5), 27-29, 49.
Mason, B., Vanata, M., Jander, K., Borsch, R., & Krashen, S. (2009). The effects and efficiency of hearing stories on vocabulary acquisition by students of German as a second foreign language in Japan. The Indonesian Journal, 5(1), 1-14.
Mason, B., Smith, K., & Krashen, S. (2020). Story-Listening in Indonesia: A replication study. Journal of English Language Teaching, 62(1), 3-6. McQuillan, J. (2016). What can readers read after graded readers? Reading in a Foreign Language, 28, 63-78.
McQuillan, J. (2019). We don’t need no stinkin’ exercises: The impact of extended instruction and storybook reading on vocabulary acquisition. Language and Language Teaching, 8(1), 25-37.
Nation, P. (2014). How much input do you need to learn the most frequent 9,000 words? Reading in a Foreign Language, 26(2), 1-16.
Smith, K. (2006). A comparison of “pure” extensive reading with supplemental activities. International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 2(2),12-15. Walter, C. (2020). 50 first stories vs. textbook: Do stories contain infrequent words? Language Issues, 1(1), 39-46.